4th September 2016
'How sharper than a serpent's tooth it is to have a thankless child'
[King Lear, I, iv]
Over the last couple of
weeks I've been inundated by literally two e-mails, both asking much the
same question:-
“Do parent ospreys recognise their returning offspring from previous seasons?”
“Do parent ospreys recognise their returning offspring from previous seasons?”
There is no simple
answer to this seemingly-straightforward issue. It seems clear that
many species of birds can recognize each other as individuals, once
they become acquainted. Of course, the term “kin-recognition”
encompasses a whole raft of responses and behaviours, but in this
article I am going to focus on the question asked, which concerns
parent-offspring recognition.
This has been proven in birds that nest communally, and the selection pressures for this ability to evolve in communal nesting are fairly obvious. Mechanisms seem to include both vocal and visual recognition. Penguin adults and chicks can locate each other – apparently by voice – even in the noise and visual confusion of a busy colony. Beecher (1988)[1] showed that bank swallows are able to do much the same thing. Birds of prey, in contrast, tend to nest by territory – but Alcaide et al (2007)[2] demonstrated that these species (ospreys included) also carry the MHC gene sequence, which had previously been associated with kin-recognition ability (and lots of other things) in mammals.
Ospreys, at best, nest semi-communally and their migratory lifestyle complicates the issue still further. Their young leave the nest at the end of summer, and undergo a complete feather-replacement moult into adult plumage before returning north to their natal regions. This means that a returning youngster will be completely different in appearance, calling into question the whole principle of visual identification by the parent.
All this stuff isn't really getting us anywhere...
This has been proven in birds that nest communally, and the selection pressures for this ability to evolve in communal nesting are fairly obvious. Mechanisms seem to include both vocal and visual recognition. Penguin adults and chicks can locate each other – apparently by voice – even in the noise and visual confusion of a busy colony. Beecher (1988)[1] showed that bank swallows are able to do much the same thing. Birds of prey, in contrast, tend to nest by territory – but Alcaide et al (2007)[2] demonstrated that these species (ospreys included) also carry the MHC gene sequence, which had previously been associated with kin-recognition ability (and lots of other things) in mammals.
Ospreys, at best, nest semi-communally and their migratory lifestyle complicates the issue still further. Their young leave the nest at the end of summer, and undergo a complete feather-replacement moult into adult plumage before returning north to their natal regions. This means that a returning youngster will be completely different in appearance, calling into question the whole principle of visual identification by the parent.
All this stuff isn't really getting us anywhere...
UV on the nest (detail from original watercolour by K Davies) |
To find out more, I
turned to one of the most closely-observed young ospreys on the
planet. Yes, you've guessed it – it's UV again.
UV is the trifecta for this job: he has a high-visibility leg ring for ease of spotting, he also has a satellite tracker for detailed following, and his parents' nest at Kielder Forest Park is monitored by video recording cameras, (courtesy of Forestry Commission England.) On 1st July 2016, he landed on that nest while his mother was looking after this year's brood of his siblings. Such an event – a year-two returnee alighting on the natal nest and interacting directly with a parent – is sufficiently rare that filmed recordings of it are almost non-existent.
But we got one.
UV is the trifecta for this job: he has a high-visibility leg ring for ease of spotting, he also has a satellite tracker for detailed following, and his parents' nest at Kielder Forest Park is monitored by video recording cameras, (courtesy of Forestry Commission England.) On 1st July 2016, he landed on that nest while his mother was looking after this year's brood of his siblings. Such an event – a year-two returnee alighting on the natal nest and interacting directly with a parent – is sufficiently rare that filmed recordings of it are almost non-existent.
But we got one.
Video (c) 2016 Forestry Commission England
Used by permission |
Short though it is, this sequence repays careful study.
We know from tracking data that UV approached the general nest area
from the south-west, and that he altered course directly towards it
when about 1000m up-range. The nest female sees him approach and
goes onto the nest to defend her brood. The chicks - of which there
are four - cannot yet fly at this date, so they crouch down in
response to the female's evident alarm-calls.
UV makes several passes over and round the nest, before finally landing on it. He alights right on the edge, prepared to take the air again at a moment's notice. His mother mantles and calls in protest at his temerity, and within seconds she has chased him away.
Is there any evidence of “recognition” here? The answer has to be “no”... ALL the birds involved behave and react in exactly the same way as if the intruder had been an unrelated individual.
Expert opinion is still divided on the initial question. Some hold strictly to the view that there is no parent-offspring recognition in European ospreys. Others prefer the view that such recognition “ought” to have evolved, and that there is no evidence to confirm its absence. A few (this author included) suspect that the parent birds cannot recognise a returning youngster, but that the youngster has some instinctive awareness of the kinship.
But all are agreed on ONE thing: it doesn't make a blind bit of difference who is right! The returnee is no more welcome at a nest site that any other osprey would be, and the breeding adults would always react in exactly the same way, offspring or not.
So is that the end of the story? Not quite.
UV makes several passes over and round the nest, before finally landing on it. He alights right on the edge, prepared to take the air again at a moment's notice. His mother mantles and calls in protest at his temerity, and within seconds she has chased him away.
Is there any evidence of “recognition” here? The answer has to be “no”... ALL the birds involved behave and react in exactly the same way as if the intruder had been an unrelated individual.
Expert opinion is still divided on the initial question. Some hold strictly to the view that there is no parent-offspring recognition in European ospreys. Others prefer the view that such recognition “ought” to have evolved, and that there is no evidence to confirm its absence. A few (this author included) suspect that the parent birds cannot recognise a returning youngster, but that the youngster has some instinctive awareness of the kinship.
But all are agreed on ONE thing: it doesn't make a blind bit of difference who is right! The returnee is no more welcome at a nest site that any other osprey would be, and the breeding adults would always react in exactly the same way, offspring or not.
So is that the end of the story? Not quite.
UV nabs a free meal at Nest 2 (Forestry Commission / J Dailey |
For the remainder of
summer 2016, we continued to follow UV's activities. He remained
interested in Kielder Forest, and intruded at all the osprey nests
there on several different occasions. All the nests, that is, except
N1a... He only made one more return visit to his parent's nest and
did not land on it again. For most of the time, he maintained a
discreet distance from it while moving around the Park.
Does this mean that UV is somehow aware that the parental nest is not a proper place for him, whereas all the others are fair game for investigation? I am not sure. Only the birds know these things for certain.
And they're still not telling.
[1] “Kin recognition in birds”: Beecher, M.D. Behav Genet (1988) 18: 465. doi:10.1007/BF01065515
Does this mean that UV is somehow aware that the parental nest is not a proper place for him, whereas all the others are fair game for investigation? I am not sure. Only the birds know these things for certain.
And they're still not telling.
[1] “Kin recognition in birds”: Beecher, M.D. Behav Genet (1988) 18: 465. doi:10.1007/BF01065515
[2] “Characterization, Polymorphism, and Evolution of MHC Class II B Genes in Birds of Prey”: Alcaide, M., Edwards, S.V. & Negro, J.J. J Mol Evol (2007) 65: 541. doi:10.1007/s00239-007-9033-9
That is fascinating Paul. Thank you....
ReplyDeleteWell I can add an interesting story....I observed a three year old osprey return to his natal nest and behave as a territorial male , copulating with the female, ( who was not his mother as she had not returned from migration the previous year and was replaced by another female), and working on the nest etc for at least a week. Eventually his father returned and easily displaced him and he settled at a nearby location, and built a nest of his own. It's the only time I have seen a young bird actually try to claim his natal nest as his territory. We can't know if the territorial male recognized this young male as his offspring or not, as he behaved as he would towards any intruding male....but it was interesting that this young male TRIED to take over this territory. ( I continue to watch this male as he behaves unusually in many different circumstances) Also if they do recognize their own offspring, why would they feed any fledging that arrives in the nest...as they often do. They seem to view them all the same. Now to add another interesting story....I witnessed an adult female attack her own offspring shortly after fledge...chasing and dive bombing as if it were an intruder. It was a banded chick so I knew it was hatched on that nest. She was so aggressive towards this young one trying to get back to the nest, that it ended up injured in the ground and I had to rescue it and take it to rehab with a broken shoulder as I recall. So I suspect they do not recognize their own offspring. But let's just keep watching and documenting because this is how we learn! The questions are as important as the answers sometimes.
ReplyDeleteanessa, I am curious about that aggressive mother's treatment of the chicks before fledge & other chicks after fledge. Was she aggressive then too. The nest at Woods Hole, MA had an aggressive mother in 2014 & 2015. The camera was not active this year.
DeleteHi Vanessa I witnessed something very similar. In 2015 when the first chick fledged it would be aggressively chased and harrassed untill it returned to the nest but this time by the male(father) and not the female. This only lasted for a day and it didn't happen to the other fledglings.
DeleteThis comment has been removed by the author.
ReplyDeleteI agree that the adults wouldn't recognize their young. Add to the plumage difference, the full extra year that the young spend down on the wintering grounds. Very few of my returning tagged juveniles have gone anywhere near their natal nests in eastern N.A. (www.ospreytrax.com)
ReplyDelete